Inoculation Theory

Kainan Jarrette and Nina Kotova

Inoculation Theory

Learning Objectives

  • Explain inoculation theory and how it relates to disinformation/misinformation.

Introduction

a syringe outline

The concept of prebunking is essentially an offshoot of a broader proactive idea: inoculation theory.

Inoculation theory is a social psychological theory that explains how an idea or belief can be protected against persuasive influence. It was developed by social scientist William J. McGuire in the 1960s, and in many ways was based on the idea of medical vaccination (leading him to call it a “vaccine for brainwashing“). Biologically speaking, if you inject a weak form of a virus into someone, their immune system will build specific defenses against that virus, and be prepared for a stronger form of attack from that virus in the future. The same holds true for inoculation theory. Although we’ll discuss it in more detail below, the basic process of the theory is this:

  1. Expose a person to a weak form of misinformation that won’t be persuasive, yet will make them defensive of their own belief on that topic
  2. That person will then likely seek out further information to strengthen the position of theirs that they feel was threatened
  3. That increased research, in turn, makes that person’s belief resistant to even stronger forms of misinformation
Several people with red umbrellas walk on a rainy sidewalk in Madrid, Spain. Across the street from them is a white cathedral.
Having a good understanding of a topic, as well as being aware of the oppositional arguments, can help one avoid misinformation or disinformation in the same way that an umbrella can help in a rainstorm.

Key Questions

  • What are the most important aspects of effective inoculation?
  • What are some advantages and limitations of inoculation theory?

Inoculation Theory in Detail

Section 1.1: Key Components

The central idea behind inoculation theory is that the messages do not supply a supportive defense, or one designed to provide people with more evidence that what they believe is true. Rather, they provide a refutational defense, or one designed to raise counterarguments to their beliefs that they may face in the future and then provide either evidence or tools to help refute those arguments. To that end, inoculation theory has two primary components that make up a successful inoculation: threat and refutational preemption.

Threat refers to the inoculation message’s ability to make a recipient aware their position or belief is vulnerable. This is usually accomplished either through presenting direct counterarguments or presenting a warning of a future attack. The important part is that the recipient of the message feels their belief is in danger, as this provides the motivation for the next component.

Refutational preemption was defined by McGuire & Papageorgis, Pfau, et al (1990) and requires that the same inoculation message that delivers the threat also provide refutations of those counterarguments. To quote directly from the authors, an effective inoculation message should “raise the specter of content potentially damaging to the receiver’s attitude while simultaneously providing direct refutation of that content in the presence of a supportive environment.”

Refutations are also categorized into two types: refutational same and refutational different (McGuire, 1964). Refutational same messages directly refute a predicted counterargument, whereas refutational different messages refute something related-but-different to the predicted counterargument. We’ll illustrate this further in an example below.

There are also two potential secondary components that are still in some debate: delay and involvement.

Delay refers to the length of time between the initial inoculation message and the next attack on that person’s belief. So far, research seems to show that there are both negative and positive consequences of delay. There does, as McGuire (1961) predicted, seem to be a certain, smaller level of delay that most optimally strengthens a belief. Essentially, you need to allow a little time for someone to do research after the initial message and let that new information sink in. However, other research also finds that the effectiveness of the initial inoculation message can begin to weaken after too long a delay (Banas & Rains, 2010).

Involvement was introduced as a concept by Pfau et al (1997) and refers to how important the attitude or belief in question is to a person. The researchers found that the effectiveness of the inoculation is strongly tied to the involvement level of the person. If an inoculation message pertained to a subject or belief of low involvement, counterarguments weren’t perceived as a threat in the way necessary for the inoculation to work. In other words, you can’t inoculate people to attacks on something they don’t care about.

Section 1.2: Studies and Examples

To get a better idea of this theory in play, we can look at a study done by Pfau et al in 1992, which involved inoculation messages aimed at preventing adolescent smoking. The general structure of the inoculation message was this:

  • First, a general warning was given that any negative feelings the adolescent participants might have about smoking would likely be challenged by peer pressure.
  • Next, the participants were shown specific potential counterarguments they might face (such as “Smoking isn’t really bad for you”).
  • Lastly, participants were shown specific refutations to those arguments (such as “Actually smoking is harmful in a number of ways, such as…”).

The results were interesting, showing that the inoculation was mostly only effective for adolescents with low self-esteem. However, adolescents with low-self esteem are at the highest risk of starting smoking, so the inoculation worked with those it most needed to (Kohsravi, 2016).

Pfau et al (1990) also developed a study during the 1988 presidential election that highlights the concept of refutational same and refutational different messaging. Republicans were claiming that the Democratic candidate was soft on crime. The refutational same message they created focused on the fact that the Democratic candidate actually advocated for harsh sentences, but also that harsh sentencing alone wouldn’t fix the crime issue. The refutational different message didn’t address that specific claim so much as they expanded on the Democratic candidate’s platforms and immediate goals. So, the former was refuting the claim he was soft on crime, while the latter was refuting the broader claim that he was a bad candidate. Interestingly, the refutational same and refutational different messages were equally effective on participants.

Section 1.3: Drawbacks and Challenges

Although Inoculation Theory and its offshoots can be quite effective, it still faces its own drawbacks (Pilditch et al, 2022):

  • As mentioned above and in the chapter on Prebunking, the effects of inoculation can wear off over time. Much like a biological vaccine, most people will need occasional “boosters” to retain the efficacy of the inoculation.
  • Adjacent to the concept of Involvement, inoculation is most effective when the participant already has a strong and accurate belief about the subject in question. The idea is to strengthen already existing ideas and beliefs, not to create new ones where none existed. Therefore, it’s important that education comes from more than just intervention techniques.

Inoculation theory also faces a somewhat ironic challenge: the same process that can be used to strengthen accurate or true beliefs can also be used to strengthen inaccurate or false beliefs. The idea of creating a “weakened” form of an argument for the sake of an inoculation message can easily be distorted into a full on straw man argument. In other words, in the strictest sense, inoculation theory works whether or not the information is correct. This is all the more reason to have separate strategies for educating people about information literacy, logical fallacies, and general critical thinking skills.

Conclusion

Despite the challenges, Inoculation Theory is overall a very effective tool for fighting misinformation. Technology has made massive advancements since the time when McGuire first introduced the theory, and as a result, more complex forms of inoculation have been developed. Audio and video messages are easier to create and spread than ever before, as well as more interactive forms of media that can engage people on an even deeper level.

Inoculation Theory has also spawned incredibly helpful alternate versions, such as prebunking. Whereas traditional Inoculation Theory relies more or less on refuting specific arguments about a topic, prebunking broadens its scope and aims to tackle the structures and tactics commonly found in misinformation messages. This greater scope creates an even bigger umbrella effect, especially as misinformation tactics are often similar, even when the content is quite different.

Further research can help discover the most effective methods of creating inoculation messages, getting those messages out to who needs them, and retaining the effects. Long-form storytelling, humor, and more direct interaction may all prove to be helpful tools in the fight against misinformation.

Key Terms

delay

the length of time between the initial inoculation message and the next attack on that person’s belief

Inoculation Theory

a social psychological theory that explains how an idea or belief can be protected against persuasive influence

involvement

how important an attitude or belief in question is to a person

refutational different

a message that refutes a different-but-related counterargument

refutational preemption

the same inoculation message that delivers the threat also provide refutations of those counterarguments

refutational same

a message that directly refutes an anticipated counterargument

threat

the inoculation message’s ability to make a recipient aware their position or belief is vulnerable

References

Banas, J. A., & Rains, S. A. (2010). A Meta-Analysis of Research on Inoculation Theory. Communication Monographs, 77(3), 281–311. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751003758193

Ecker, U. K. H., Lewandowsky, S., Cook, J., Schmid, P., Fazio, L. K., Brashier, N., Kendeou, P., Vraga, E. K., & Amazeen, M. A. (2022). The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-021-00006-y

Harjani, T., Roozenbeek, J., Biddlestone, M., van der Linden, S., Stuart, A., Iwahara, M., Piri, B., Xu, R., Goldberg, B., & Graham, M. (2022). A Practical Guide  to Prebunking Misinformation. https://interventions.withgoogle.com//#prebunking

Khosravi, A., Mohammadpoorasl, A., Holakouie-Naieni, K., Mahmoodi, M., Pouyan, A. A., & Mansournia, M. A. (2016). Causal Effect of Self-esteem on Cigarette Smoking Stages in Adolescents: Coarsened Exact Matching in a Longitudinal Study. Osong public health and research perspectives, 7(6), 341–345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.phrp.2016.10.003

McGuire, W. J. (1961). Resistance to persuasion conferred by active and passive prior refutation of the same and alternative counterarguments. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(2), 326–332. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048344

McGuire W. J. (1964). Inducing resistance to persuasion: some contemporary approaches. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 191–229.

Pfau, M., Bockern, S. V., & Kang, J. G. (1992). Use of inoculation to promote resistance to smoking initiation among adolescents. Communication Monographs, 59(3), 213–230. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759209376266

Pfau, M., Kenski, H. C., Nitz, M., & Sorenson, J. (1990). Efficacy of inoculation strategies in promoting resistance to political attack messages: Application to direct mail. Communication Monographs, 57(1), 25–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759009376183

Pfau M., Tusing K. J., Koerner A. F., Lee W., Godbold L. C., Penaloza L. J., et al. (1997). Enriching the inoculation construct: the role of critical components in the process of resistance. Hum. Commun. Res. 24, 187–215. 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1997.tb00413.x

Pilditch, T. D., Roozenbeek, J., Madsen, J. K., & van der Linden, S. (2022). Psychological inoculation can reduce susceptibility to misinformation in large rational agent networks. Royal Society open science, 9(8), 211953. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.211953

Roozenbeek, J., Traberg, C. S., & van der Linden, S. (2022). Technique-based inoculation against real-world misinformation. Royal Society Open Science, 9(5), 211719. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.211719

Roozenbeek, J., & Van Der Linden, S. (2021). Inoculation theory and misinformation. NATO Strategic Communications Centre of Excellence. https://stratcomcoe.org/publications/inoculation-theory-and-misinformation/217

 

Media Attributions

definition

License

Icon for the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License

Immersive Truth Copyright © by Kainan Jarrette and Nina Kotova is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License, except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book